Peer Reviewed Research Papers (*advised student/postdoc first author)
Memory
Recognition
Memory
- *Potter, K.
W., Huszar, L. D., & Huber, D. E. (2018). Does inhibition cause forgetting after selective retrieval? A reanalysis and
failure to replicate. Cortex, 26-45.
- *Jang, Y., Wixted, J. T.,
& Huber, D. E. (2011). The diagnosticity of individual data for model selection:
Comparing signal-detection models of recognition memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18,
751-757. (supplementary material).
- *Jang, Y., Wixted, J.,
& Huber, D. E. (2009).
Testing signal-detection models of yes/no and two-alternative
forced-choice recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General, 138, 291-306.
- Huber, D. E., Clark, T. F., Curran, T., & Winkielman, P. (2008). Effects of repetition
priming on recognition memory: Testing a perceptual fluency-disfluency
model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition , 34, 1305-1324.
- Shiffrin,
R. M., Huber, D. E., & Marinelli, K. (1995). Effects of category
length and strength on familiarity in recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, &
Cognition, Vol. 21, No. 2, 267-287.
- Huber,
D. E., Marinelli, K., Ziemer, H. E., &
Shiffrin, R. M. (1992). Does memory activation
grow with list strength and/or length? Proceedings
of the 14th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, (pp
147-152). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates. [40% acceptance rate].
- Nobel,
P. A. & Huber, D. E. (1993). Modeling
forced-choice associative recognition through a hybrid of global
recognition and cued-recall. Proceedings
of the 15th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, (pp
783-788). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.[22%
acceptance rate].
Recall,
Testing Effects, and Metamemory
- *Aenugu, S. & Huber, D. E. (2021). Asymmetric weights and
retrieval practice in an autoassociative neural
network model of paired-associate learning. Neural Computation, 33,
3351-3360.
- *Hopper,
W. J. & Huber, D. E. (2019).
Testing the Primary and Convergent Retrieval model of recall: Recall
practice produces faster recall success but also faster recall failure. Memory and Cognition, 47, 816-841.
- Jang,
Y., Lee, H., & Huber, D. E. (2019). How
many dimensions underlie judgments of learning and recall redux:
Consideration of recall latency reveals a previously hidden
nonmonotonicity. Journal of
Mathematical Psychology, 90,
47-60.
- *Hopper, W. J., & Huber, D.
E. (2018). Learning to recall:
Examining recall latencies to test an intra-item learning theory of
testing effects. Journal of Memory
and Language, 102, 1-15.
- *Hopper, W. J., Huber, D. E. (2016). The primary and convergent
retrieval model of recall. Papafragou, A., Grodner, D., Mirman, D., & Trueswell,
J.C. (Eds.). Proceedings of the 38th
Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Austin, TX:
Cognitive Science Society, 1235-1240.
- Huber, D.
E., Tomlinson, T. D., Jang, Y., & Hopper, W. J. (2015). The
search of associative memory with recovery interference (SAM-RI) memory
model and its application to retrieval practice paradigms. In J. Raaijmakers, A. Criss, R. Goldstone, R. Nosofsky, & M. Steyvers
(Eds.) Cognitive Modeling in Perception and Memory: A Festschrift for
Richard M. Shiffrin. New York: Psychology Press, 81-98.
- *Jang, Y., Pashler, H., & Huber, D. E. (2014). Manipulations of choice familiarity
in multiple-choice testing support a retrieval practice account of the
testing effect. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106(2),
435-447.
- *Jang, Y., Wallsten, T. S., & Huber, D. E. (2012). A stochastic detection and retrieval
model for the study of metacognition. Psychological Review, 119(1),
186-200. (supplemental material).
- *Jang, Y., Wixted, J. T., Pecher, D., Zeelenberg, R.,
& Huber, D. E. (2012).
Decomposing the interaction
between retention interval and study/test practice: The role of
retrievability. Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 65(5), 962-975.
- Huber, D. E., Tomlinson, T. D., Rieth,
C. A., & Davelaar, E. J. (2010). Reply to Bauml
and Hanslmayr: Adding or subtracting memories?
The neural correlates of learned interference versus memory inhibition. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 107(2), E4.
- *Tomlinson, T. D., Huber, D. E., Rieth,
C. A., & Davelaar, E. J. (2009). An interference account of
cue-independent forgetting in the no-think paradigm. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 15588-15593.
(supporting material).
- *Jang, Y.
& Huber, D. E. (2008). Context retrieval and context change in free recall: Recalling
from long-term memory drives list isolation. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 34, 112-127.
Perception
Letter/Word
Identification
- *Potter, K. W.,
Donkin, C., & Huber, D. E. (2018).
The elimination of positive priming with increasing prime duration
reflects a transition from perceptual fluency to disfluency rather than
bias against primed words. Cognitive Psychology, 101, 1-28.
- *Rieth, C. A. & Huber, D. E. (2017). Comparing different kinds of words
and word-word relations to test an habituation
model of priming. Cognitive Psychology, 95, 79-104.
- Liu, J., Li,
J., Rieth, C. A., Huber, D. E., Tian, J, &
Lee, K,. (2011). A dynamic causal modeling analysis of the effective connectivities underlying top-down letter processing. Neuropsychologia, 49(5), 1177-1186.
- Liu, J., Li,
J., Liang, J., Zhang, H., Rieth, C. A., Huber,
D. E., Lee, K, & Tian, J. (2010). Neural correlates of top-down letter processing. Neuropsychologia. 48, 636-641.
- Huber, D. E.
(2008). Immediate
Priming and Cognitive Aftereffects. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General, 137, 324-347.
- Huber, D. E., Tian, X., Curran, T., O'Reilly, C,
& Woroch, B. (2008). The dynamics of
integration and separation: ERP, MEG, and neural network studies of
immediate repetition effects. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(6), 1389-1416.
- *Weidemann, C. T., Huber,
D. E., Shiffrin, R. M. (2008). Prime diagnosticity in short-term repetition priming: Is
primed evidence discounted even when it reliably indicates the correct
answer? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and
Cognition, 34(2), 257-281.
- *Weidemann, C. T., Huber, D. E., Shiffrin, R. M. (2005). Confusion and compensation in
visual perception: Effects of spatiotemporal proximity and selective
attention. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 40-61.
- Huber, D. E. & Cousineau, D. (2004). A race model of perceptual forced choice
reaction time. In R. Alterman & D. Kirsh
(Eds.), Proceedings of the 25th Annual
Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. (pp 593-598). Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum Associates. [24% acceptance rate].
- Huber,
D. E. & O'Reilly,
R. C. (2003). Persistence and
accommodation in short-term priming and other perceptual paradigms:
Temporal segregation through synaptic depression. Cognitive Science: A
Multidisciplinary Journal, 27, 403-430. Appendix.
- Colagrosso, M. D., Mozer, M. C., & Huber, D. E. (2003).
Mechanisms of skill refinement: A model of long-term repetition priming.
In R. Alterman & D. Kirsh (Eds.), Proceedings of the 25th Annual Conference
of the Cognitive Science Society. (pp 316-321). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Associates. [24% acceptance rate].
- Wagenmakers, E. M., Zeelenberg,
R., Huber, D. E., Raaijmakers, J. G. W.,
Shiffrin, R. M., & Schooler, L. J. (2003). REMI and
ROUSE: Quantitative Models for Long-Term and Short-Term Priming in
Perceptual Identification. In Marsolek, C. J,
& Bowers, J. S. (Eds.), Rethinking Implicit Memory. Oxford
University Press.
- Huber,
D. E., Shiffrin, R. M., Lyle, K. B., & Quach, R. (2002). Mechanisms of source
confusion and discounting in short-term priming 2: Effects of prime
similarity and target duration. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Learning Memory and Cognition, 28, 1120-1136.
- Huber,
D. E., Shiffrin, R. M., Quach, R., & Lyle, K. B. (2002). Mechanisms of source
confusion and discounting in short-term priming 1: Effects of prime
duration and prime recognition. Memory & Cognition, 30,
745-757.
- Huber,
D. E., Shiffrin, R. M., Lyle, K. B., & Ruys,
K. I. (2001). Perception
and preference in short-term word priming. Psychological Review, 108(1),
149-182.
- Huber,
D. E. (2000). Perception and preference in
short-term word priming. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Indiana
University, Bloomington.
Semantics
- *Jacob,
L. P. L. & Huber, D. E. (2019). Neural
habituation enhances novelty detection: an EEG study of rapidly presented
words. Computational Brain &
Behavior, 1-20.
- *Tian, X.
& Huber, D. E. (2013). Playing 'duck
duck goose' with neurons: Change detection through
connectivity reduction. Psychological Science, 24(6),
819-827. (supplementary material).
- *Smith, K.
A., Huber, D. E., & Vul, E. (2013). Multiply-constrained semantic search
in the Remote Associates Test. Cognition, 128, 64-75.
- *Gupta, N.,
Jang, Y., Mednick, S. C., & Huber, D. E. (2012). The road not taken: Creative
solutions require avoidance of high frequency responses. Psychological
Science, 23(3), 288-294. (supplementary material).
- Pecher, D.,
Boot, I., van Dantzig, S., Madden, C. J., Huber, D. E., & Zeelenberg, R. (2011). The sound of enemies and friends in the neighborhood: Phonology
mediates activation of neighbor semantics. Experimental Psychology,
58(6), 454-463.
- *Tian, X.
& Huber, D. E. (2010). Testing an associative account of semantic satiation. Cognitive
Psychology, 60, 267-290.
- Pecher, D.,
Van Dantzig, S., Boot, I., Zanolie, K., &
Huber, D. E. (2010). Congruency between word position and meaning is caused by task
induced spatial attention. Frontiers in Cognition, 1, article 30, 8
pages.
Visual
Identification of Faces, Objects, and Orientated Lines
- *Jacob,
L. P. L., Potter, K. W., & Huber, D. E. (2021). A neural habituation
account of the negative compatibility effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General.
- *Sadil, P. S., Potter, K. W., Huber, D. E., &
Cowell, R. A. (2019). Connecting the
dots without top-down knowledge: Evidence for rapidly-learned low-level
associations that are independent of object identity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(6), 1058-1070.
- Carr, E.
W., Huber, D. E., Pecher, D., Zeelenberg, R., Halberstadt,
J., & Winkielman, P. (2017). The ugliness-in-averageness effect:
Tempering the warm glow of familiarity. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 112(6). 787-812.
- *Hopper, W.
J., Finklea, K. M., Winkielman,
P., & Huber, D. E. (2014). Measuring sexual dimorphism with a race-gender face space. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Human Perception and Performance, 40(5), 1779-1788.
- *Rieth, C. A., Lee, K., Liu, J., Tian, K., & Huber,
D. E. (2011). Faces in the mist: Illusory face and letter detection. i-Perception, 2, 458-476.
- *Rieth, C. A. & Huber,
D. E. (2010). Priming and habituation for faces: Individual
differences and inversion effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and
Performance, 36, 596-618.
- Li, J., Liu, J., Liang, J., Zhang, H., Zhao, J., Rieth, C. A.,
Huber, D. E., Li, W., Shi, G., Ai, L., Tian, J., & Lee, K. (2010). Effective connectivities
of cortical regions for top-down face processing: A dynamic causal
modeling study. Brain Research, 1340, 40-51.
- Li, J., Liu,
J., Liang, J., Zhang, H., Zhao, J., Huber, D. E., Rieth,
C. A., Lee, K., Tian, J., & Shi, G. (2009). A distributed neural system for
top-down face processing. Neuroscience Letters, 451, 6-10.
- Cowell,
R. A., Huber, D. E., & Cottrell, G. W. (2009). Virtual Brain Reading: A
Connectionist Approach to Understanding fMRI. Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive
Science Society. [32% acceptance rate]. 212-217.
- Zhang,
H., Liu, J., Huber, D. E., Rieth, C., Tian, J., & Lee, K. (2008). Detecting faces in pure noise images: An fMRI study on top-down
perception. Neuroreport, 19,
229-233.
- *Rieth, C. A. & Huber,
D. E. (2005).
Using a neural network model with synaptic
depression to assess the dynamics of feature-based versus configural
processing in face identification. Proceedings of the 27th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science
Society. (pp 1856-1861). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates. [26%
acceptance rate].
Attention and Cognitive Control
- Wessel,
J. R. & Huber, D. E. (2019). Frontal
cortex tracks surprise separately for different sensory modalities but
engages a common inhibitory control mechanism. PLOS Computational Biology, 15(7), 26 pages.
- *Huszar, L.
D., & Huber, D. E. (2018).
Evidence that the Attention Blink Reflects Categorical Perceptual
Dynamics. Kalish, C., Rau, M., Zhu, J., & Rogers, T. (Eds.). Proceedings of the 40th Annual
Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Madison, WI: Cognitive
Science Society, 1847-1852.
- *Rusconi, P.
& Huber, D. E. (2018). The perceptual wink model of non-switching attentional blink
tasks. Psychonomic Bulletin
& Review, 25, 1717-1739.
- *Rieth, C. A. & Huber, D. E. (2013). Implicit learning of spatiotemporal
contingencies in spatial cueing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and
Performance, 39, 1165-1180.
- *Davelaar, E. J., Tian, X., Weidemann,
C. T., & Huber, D. E. (2011). A habituation account of change detection in same/different
judgments. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 11,
608-626. (supplementary material).
- Mozer, M. C., Colagrosso, M.
D., & Huber, D. E. (2002).
A rational analysis of cognitive control in a speeded discrimination task.
In T. Dietterich, S. Becker, & Ghahramani, Z. (Eds.) Advances in Neural
Information Processing Systems XIV (pp. 51-57). Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press. [30% acceptance rate; impact rating (CiteSeer):
1.06, top 20.96%].
Social Cognition
- *Siegel, E.,
Dougherty, M. R., & Huber, D. E. (2012). Manipulating the need for cognitive
control while taking the implicit association test. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 1057-1068.
- *Siegel, E.,
Sigall, H., & Huber, D. E. (2012). The IAT is sensitive to the
perceived accuracy of newly learned associations. European Journal of
Social Psychology, 42, 189-199.
- Winkielman,
P., Huber, D. E., Kavanagh, L., & Schwarz, N. (2012). Fluency
of consistency: When thoughts fit nicely and flow smoothly. In B.
Gawronski & F. Strack (Eds.) Cognitive
consistency: A fundamental principle in social cognition. New York: Guilford
Press. 89-111.
- Winkielman, P., Huber, D.
E., & Olszanowski, M. (2011).
Dynamic connections: The role of processing fluency in affect and
evaluation. In Blaszczak, W
& Dolinski, D. (Eds.) Dynamics
of emotion: Theory and applications. PWN. Warsaw. 60-87. [in Polish].
- *Irwin, K.
R., Huber, D. E., & Winkielman, P. (2010). Automatic Affective Dynamics: An
activation-habituation model of affective assimilation and contrast. In
Nishida, T., Jain, L. C., & Faucher, C.
(Eds.) Modelling Machine Emotions for Realizing Intelligence:
Foundations and Applications. Springer Verlag. 17-34.
- Winkielman, P. & Huber, D. E. (2009).
Dynamics and evaluation: The warm glow of processing fluency. In Meyers,
R. A. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of
Complexity and Systems Science. New York: Springer Verlag. Part 4,
2242-2253.
Theory, Models, and Methods
- Sadil, P., Cowell, R. A., & Huber, D. E. (2022). A modeling framework for
determining modulation of neural-level tuning from non-invasive human fMRI
data. Communications Biology. (Supplementary Information).
- Park,
J., & Huber, D. E. (2022).
A visual sense of number emerges from divisive normalization in a simple
center-surround convolutional network. eLife.
1-16.
- Huber,
D. E., Cohen, A. L., & Staub, A. (2022).
A 'compensatory selection' effect with standardized tests: Lack of
correlation between test scores and success is evidence that test scores
are predictive of success. PLOS ONE.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0265459
- Cowell,
R. A. & Huber, D. E. (2020).
Mechanisms of memory: An intermediate level of analysis and organization. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences,
32, 65-71.
- *Sadil, P. S., Cowell, R. A., & Huber, D. E. (2019). A hierarchical Bayesian state trace
analysis for assessing monotonicity while factoring out subject, item, and
trial level dependencies. Journal of
Mathematical Psychology, 90,
118-131.
- Huber,
D. E., Potter, K. W., & Huszar, L. D. (2019). Less "Story" and more "Reliability" in
cognitive neuroscience. Cortex, 113, 347-349.
- Huber, D. E.
(2015). Using
continual flash suppression to investigate cognitive aftereffects. Consciousness
and Cognition, 35, 30-32.
- Huber, D. E.
(2014). The
rise and fall of the recent past: a unified account of immediate
repetition paradigms. In B. Ross (Ed.) Psychology of Learning and
Motivation, 60. PLM, UK: Academic Press, 191-226. (integration and separation demo).
- *Tian, X., Poeppel, D., & Huber, D. E. (2011). TopoToolbox:
Using sensor topography to calculate psychologically meaningful measures
from event-related EEG/MEG. Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience.
Article ID 674605, 8 pages.
- Huber, D. E. & Cowell, R. A. (2010). Theory driven modeling or model
driven theorizing? Comment on McClelland et al/Griffiths et al. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(8), 343-344.
- Huber, D. E. (2008). Causality in time: Explaining away
the future and the past. In M. Oaksford and N. Chater (Eds.). The probabilistic mind: Prospects
for rational models of cognition. Oxford University Press. 351-376.
- *Tian, X.
& Huber, D. E. (2008). Measures of spatial similarity and response magnitude in MEG
and scalp EEG. Brain Topography, 20(3), 131-141.
- Huber, D. E. (2006).
Computer simulations of the ROUSE model: an analytic method and generally
applicable techniques for producing parameter confidence intervals. Behavior Research Methods, 38,
557-568.
- Huber,
D. E. (1998). The development of synchrony
between oscillating neurons. Proceedings
of the 20th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, (502-507).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates. [acceptance rate not available].